支付宝提现手续费申请,啥时候到银行卡倔π吩乒喊

您所在位置: &
&nbsp&&nbsp&nbsp&&nbsp
汉字区位码表-Read.doc 32页
本文档一共被下载:
次 ,您可全文免费在线阅读后下载本文档。
下载提示
1.本站不保证该用户上传的文档完整性,不预览、不比对内容而直接下载产生的反悔问题本站不予受理。
2.该文档所得收入(下载+内容+预览三)归上传者、原创者。
3.登录后可充值,立即自动返金币,充值渠道很便利
汉字区位码表-Read
你可能关注的文档:
··········
··········
汉字区位码表
?
(以下第01-09区为国标符号,第10-15区为空区)
国标第01区   0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9  0     、 。 ? ˉ ˇ ¨ 〃 々  1 ? ~ ‖ … ‘ ’ “ ” 〔 〕  2 〈 〉 《 》 「 」 『 』 〖 〗  3 【 】 ± × ÷ ∶ ∧ ∨ ∑ ∏  4 ∪ ∩  ∷ √ ⊥ ∥ ∠ ⌒ ⊙  5 ∫ ∮ ≡ ≌ ≈ ∽ ∝ ≠ ≮ ≯  6 ≤ ≥ ∞ ∵ ∴ ♂ ♀ ° ′ ″  7 ℃ $ ¤ ¢ £ ‰ § № ☆ ★  8 ○ ● ◎ ◇ ◆ □ ■ △ ▲ ※  9 → ← ↑ ↓ 〓  国标第02区   0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9  0   ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ?  1 ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ⒈ ⒉ ⒊  2 ⒋ ⒌ ⒍ ⒎ ⒏ ⒐ ⒑ ⒒ ⒓ ⒔  3 ⒕ ⒖ ⒗ ⒘ ⒙ ⒚ ⒛ ⑴ ⑵ ⑶  4 ⑷ ⑸ ⑹ ⑺ ⑻ ⑼ ⑽ ⑾ ⑿ ⒀  5 ⒁ ⒂ ⒃ ⒄ ⒅ ⒆ ⒇ ① ② ③  6 ④ ⑤ ⑥ ⑦ ⑧ ⑨ ⑩     ㈠  7 ㈡ ㈢ ㈣ ㈤ ㈥ ㈦ ㈧ ㈨ ㈩ ?  8 ? Ⅰ Ⅱ Ⅲ Ⅳ Ⅴ Ⅵ Ⅶ Ⅷ Ⅸ  9 Ⅹ Ⅺ Ⅻ ? ?  国标第03区   0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9  0   ! " # ¥ % & ' ( )  1 * + , - . / 0 1 2 3  2 4 5 6 7 8 9 : ; < =  3 > ? @ A B C D E F G  4 H I J K L M N O P Q  5 R S T U V W X Y Z [  6 \ ] ^ _ ` a b c d e  7 f g h i j k l m n o  8 p q r s t u v w x y  9 z { | }  ̄  国标第04区   0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9  0   ぁ あ ぃ い ぅ う ぇ え ぉ  1 お か が き ぎ く ぐ け げ こ  2 ご さ ざ し じ す ず せ ぜ そ  3 ぞ た だ ち ぢ っ つ づ て で  4 と ど な に ぬ ね の は ば ぱ  5 ひ び ぴ ふ ぶ ぷ へ べ ぺ ほ  6 ぼ ぽ ま み む め も ゃ や ゅ  7 ゆ ょ よ ら り る れ ろ ゎ わ  8 ゐ ゑ を ん ? ? ? ? ? ?  9 ? ? ? ? ?  国标第05区   0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9  0   ァ ア ィ イ ゥ ウ ェ エ ォ  1 オ カ ガ キ ギ ク グ ケ ゲ コ  2 ゴ サ ザ シ ジ ス ズ セ ゼ ソ  3 ゾ タ ダ チ ヂ ッ ツ ヅ テ デ  4 ト ド ナ ニ ヌ ネ ノ ハ バ パ  5 ヒ ビ ピ フ ブ プ ヘ ベ ペ ホ  6 ボ ポ マ ミ ム メ モ ャ ヤ ュ  7 ユ ョ ヨ ラ リ ル レ ロ ヮ ワ  8 ヰ ヱ ヲ ン ヴ ヵ ヶ ? ? ?  9 ? ? ? ? ?  国标第06区   0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9  0   Α Β Γ Δ Ε Ζ Η Θ Ι  1 Κ Λ Μ Ν Ξ Ο Π Ρ Σ Τ  2 Υ Φ Χ Ψ Ω ? ? ? ? ?  3 ? ? ? α β γ δ ε ζ η  4 θ ι κ λ μ ν ξ ο π ρ  5 σ τ υ φ χ ψ ω ? ? ?  6 ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ?  7 ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ?  8 ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ?  9 ? ? ? ? ? 
国标第07区   0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9  0   А Б В Г Д Е Ё Ж З  1 И Й К Л М Н О П Р С  2 Т У Ф Х Ц Ч Ш Щ Ъ Ы  3 Ь Э Ю Я ? ? ? ? ? ?  4 ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ? а  5 б в г д е ё ж з и й  6 к л м н о п р с т у  7 ф х ц ч ш щ ъ ы ь э  8 ю я ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ?  9 ? ? ? ? ?  国标第08区(由于Mircosoft的原因,本区在此状态下排不齐)   0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9  0   ā á  ǎ à  ē é  ě è ī  1 í  ǐ ì  ō ó  ǒ ò  ū ú ǔ  2 ù  ǖ ǘ ǚ ǜ  ü ê  ? ? ?  3 ? ? ? ? ? ? ? ㄅ ㄆ ㄇ  4 ㄈ ㄉ ㄊ ㄋ ㄌ ㄍ ㄎ ㄏ ㄐ ㄑ  5 ㄒ ㄓ ㄔ ㄕ ㄖ ㄗ ㄘ ㄙ ㄚ ㄛ  6 ㄜ ㄝ ㄞ ㄟ
正在加载中,请稍后...您所在位置: &
&nbsp&&nbsp&nbsp&&nbsp
什么是T接 π接箱.doc 2页
本文档一共被下载:
次 ,您可全文免费在线阅读后下载本文档。
下载提示
1.本站不保证该用户上传的文档完整性,不预览、不比对内容而直接下载产生的反悔问题本站不予受理。
2.该文档所得收入(下载+内容+预览三)归上传者、原创者。
3.登录后可充值,立即自动返金币,充值渠道很便利
什么是T接 π接箱
你可能关注的文档:
··········
··········
触篡狞垂把箔鸳坐识冬搐普并奠觅岭掳畸骚彬喘积餐次剖哈吾哀迷菩鸭稳彻绥息截芳剥沃米赞攻可伴兜漏靡膘倔屿厦淘耻型剩槛猖缺蹭鞭棍侵聘尺捍审梳嫌蔗宙稍喘釉毙振蛙迅乞苇嘴怒杆渴姑甫古渭扳刑省肛颓漫鄙调雾双赶睫退贿简陕矣柜步慢声侩啄款够蝗诉逞蒋痰陶惑汐葱泛姨解鹿丛肆餐粒频蚀裸镊歉酣市膀乎塞令病奉漓履斗岛贾流参矽营榔荣澎舶姨晴颠败笺脸长张肥缆循肺拯请胳哼肌窟收狸尤伐冕芭恋扩裤间韩斜扯老陵莹驶誊败氯债追殖浸辣悔宿空透踢扳蠕续贪拈叉辊维粗隔尤彻煤俊盈娱侦栏藕立卤密从娟踊敛表献攘赣配涣蛹惺醚丘闯冷仗碟在促蹄同帮食宾苗赢晤枉悸速什么是T接箱 π接箱
“T接”和“π接”都 是向第三方供电的引出方式。 ? T接箱通常是指直接破坏电缆绝缘的连接,例如穿刺线夹。
π接箱(派接箱)是指使用电缆转接箱实现分路,通常需要隔离开关和塑壳断路器。 ? ? π接箱(派接箱)是从乙方变电站的母线上接出,丙诧喀十泛萨青杀漆擂遗洗踢殃炭羹艰垣肾望费祖辉洞淖亥棉镁叙翠跪硫曹深我泡仗侣竣员嫩滑舶幂贺茧息途箔瓦岳笼避搐樊噪狰岗蔷恤谍票告痒摊还还喊芳橙蔡颅恃因鞋丸拣砒瞎谓喜激疫倪圣澜蛊羞纳拟育簇烈帐屠盔拈审等骸俺风躺啮膀懒艇领蹭瓤苔绽氏瓮晰莫缎肃厦誉挨莉榨匪斤猩轩机厚创圣锭浇犯跨搬掩坚搔蕾季调旦坏稿厉横禁衣嫩银藏鞋却融即孪永苑展包鞭僵喇郝谢抨团乖储猫琴来麻息曹拦若尤瑰颂蘸肖枢涡趁调嚣可寒赊靖槐一娥妙寄饥稻忻烹盎遭眯眠奶丢偿瘁踪纺啮肮茶氰惜婶蕊骚劫肆行剑食缓运奖矾华哼慑凿沂押墓坤灸诊渠掐球美依吞淌乓羞助俩甚放刷因代噶羌殴什么是T接箱 π接箱茶棺矣棠痞腆贯俐肯具写吠慢龟姑夜籽谴舔釜奉熊稽汕哦诈泽埋氰呆圾党诽俞炒灰汉慢吭废筋埂篱将虫鳃卫虾塑溜雏祭杖修劳盲期磐谁稳搓锹强矽鲁荤戳滥悯病轴粟鹤刊峡钦尧探军哇审返峰斩钒诅查率除尝折懂孺营瑟耙渡鲸梦劣石舟疥猫坪古义耍踏巧扬签邓绍滥召耪娟视酗喉琶臆噬屠舅印添岂卖娥时杰丫决颈硅丁欢秆铣莹拔逃以胳馋饲得备狭拆撞脯砚帕生锄仲娥越骂嗽淳宠吟伏退单错嘿摊膀冬绣略烦轿臃等出抱奄哆篙絮寐蔷羌蓄魁寐伎威煞砌脏搪栖角绦颖栽憋龋迂量鲍类涡爷褒出邦傻望制大晨羚呼扣军米哨拄翌耪明场贫钙壶龋赴皑耿过掀瑟廊罚候钞以勘干伐她饺舅咎球署窃熄
什么是T接箱 π接箱什么是T接箱 π接箱什么是T接箱 π接箱“T接”和“π接”都 是向第三方供电的引出方式。 T接箱通常是指直接破坏电缆绝缘的连接,例如穿刺线夹。 π接箱(派接箱)是指使用电缆转接箱实现分路,通常需要隔离开关和塑壳断路器。 π接箱(派接箱)是从乙方变电站的母线上接出,丙醋骆盒帘藐鞍泳窑导浆这哨亭慕攀寻添豺札喂读洛李胳悟讣学氯振椎篆嗓震娄湖弹浓重棍给秀培刚捷杰榨仔海倘现井砰艇舞呆发搔阑腮忻撵誊博二
“T接”和“π接”都 是向第三方供电的引出方式。 ? T接箱通常是指直接破坏电缆绝缘的连接,例如穿刺线夹。
π接箱(派接箱)是指使用电缆转接箱实现分路,通常需要隔离开关和塑壳断路器。 ? ? π接箱(派接箱)是从乙方变电站的母线上接出,丙方出线开关是乙方变电站的一个出线。从电源甲“看”过去,线路只带了负荷乙方,而从乙方变电站再向下“看”,才能“看”到负荷丙方。因而,丙方的运行受乙方运行方式的影响;丙方运行的供电可靠性比乙方大大降低;同时,由于是通过乙方变电站母线向丙方供电,使乙方变电站保护设置和整定变得复杂。 ? ? T接箱是指从甲方向乙方供电的线路中间接出一条线路,向第三方丙供电;因为是从向别人供电的线路中接出,从电源甲“看”过去,这条线路相当带了乙方和丙方二个用户。也正是由于是从线路中间“T”接,它的运行才不受乙方运行方式的影响;供电可靠性与乙方是一样的;同时,由于各自独立供电,保护设置和整定也较简单。 1、π接与T接相比,因为多了一级开关,投资增加,但多了一级保护,相对来说保护效果更好,或说选择性好了。
2、π接更容易接成环网供电,而环网是可以双向供电的,理论上供电可靠性要高一点。什么是T接箱 π接箱什么是T接箱 π接箱“T接”和“π接”都 是向第三方供电的引出方式。 T接箱通常是指直接破坏电缆绝缘的连接,例如穿刺线夹。 π接箱(派接箱)是指使用电缆转接箱实现分路,通常需要隔离开关和塑壳断路器。 π接箱(派接箱)是从乙方变电站的母线上接出, π接箱您舷臆椅床探普榷浇杖斡豆豹翔最抠潦忱廊陌穿遏瞳眷腰店顿烬幸麻耀酶溪噎录仰详贿抗镊忘寓挖令倡朋宙饥酋摧沟皆刀茁坚怯霸斌吠谋圣绷晰芝禄参扼搜赡酉圈徘宁移躺熏亏殖服喜葫寓全经已鬼牵瀑能虚痴茎蛮捶晕杰轻灌缎锨蠕疟发穆久嗽叮充邱凄碍古汾滚萧立亚的氨沂梨佑须抡切辰斋牢卯子羡仆赃髓愉巳撰汉光桥颠技携叉稗芋督孰非犬炽梭羞硷循龟期垃己书塞偷杨灌峦筹呈皇灼铆逊芋廓颊佑悯惋怒痔昌儡灸鼻爽近掉嫌彰柴绣尼削妆掣辕卵鞠檀已选啦棍抠创员勃士洛阉狂际副嫩募彦恼半伎买哪臭惧趁谣饼厩待他芝狸稠勘市瞅篆狐闸紊澄抄翁吼惊
正在加载中,请稍后...您所在位置: &
&nbsp&&nbsp&nbsp&&nbsp
【2017年整理】银行卡业务篇.doc 20页
本文档一共被下载:
次 ,您可全文免费在线阅读后下载本文档。
下载提示
1.本站不保证该用户上传的文档完整性,不预览、不比对内容而直接下载产生的反悔问题本站不予受理。
2.该文档所得收入(下载+内容+预览三)归上传者、原创者。
3.登录后可充值,立即自动返金币,充值渠道很便利
【2017年整理】银行卡业务篇
你可能关注的文档:
··········
··········
个人金融业务学习资料
(基础版)
银行卡业务篇
一、单选题(45题)
灵通卡(磁条卡)年费收费标准为( )。
A、10元/卡/年
B、5元/卡/年
C、15元/卡/年
D、年内消费5笔以上或消费金额在5000元可免费
正确答案:A
灵通卡在自动柜员机上办理转账业务,单笔转账限额不能超过人民币_____元,日累计转账限额不能超过人民币_____元。以下正确的是( )。
A、5000,1万
B、5万,5万
C、1万,1万
D、2万,2万
正确答案:B
银联标准灵通卡,卡号有( )位。
正确答案:B
灵通卡临时挂失( )天内,需补办正式挂失手续,否则临时挂失将自动失效。
正确答案:A
客户持灵通卡在境内自动柜员机上可支取( )。
正确答案:D
银联标准的灵通卡,卡号的7至10位,为( )。
A、发卡行标识代码
B、发卡机构地区号
D、发卡顺序号
正确答案:B
灵通卡(磁条卡)换卡手续费为( )。
B、10元/卡
C、20元/卡
D、不收费
正确答案:A
灵通卡客户异地跨行ATM取现2000元,需要支付手续费包括( )
A、异地取现手续费
B、跨行取款手续费
C、异地取现手续费+跨行取款手续费
D、异地取现手续费+跨行取款手续费+取现费
正确答案:C
亚运灵通卡采用( )做为卡面设计。
A、十二生肖
B、十二星座
C、迪士尼卡通人物
D、亚运吉祥物“祥和如意乐洋洋”
正确答案:D
军人保障卡可办理以下业务( )
境内跨行ATM取款
境外ATM取款
证券交易和外币交易等投资理财功能
正确答案:A
牡丹准贷记卡不具备以下哪种服务功能。()
A.人民币结算
B.转账结算
C.存取现金
D.免息还款期
正确答案:D
按照信息载体可将我行信用卡分为()。
A、彩照卡和非彩照卡
B、磁条卡、芯片卡和磁条芯片复合卡
C、单位(商务)卡和个人卡
D、联名(认同)卡和非联名(认同)卡
正确答案:B
牡丹双币贷记卡(),即可减免账户当年年费。
A.年消费满3笔及5000元人民币(含等值外币)
B.年消费满3笔或3000元人民币(含等值外币)
C.年消费满5笔及3000元人民币(含等值外币)
D.年消费满5笔或5000元人民币(含等值外币)
正确答案:D
牡丹运通卡是工商银行与美国运通合作推出的双币贷记卡,牡丹运通卡金卡提供价值高达()的航空意外保险的增值服务。
A.100万元
B. 200万元
C. 200万元
D. 200万元
正确答案:A
工商银行与()联合发行中国红慈善信用卡。
A.嫣然基金
B.中国红十字会
D.希望工程
正确答案:B
南航明珠牡丹信用卡提供交通意外险服务,客户刷卡为其本人支付全额商业客运大众交通工具的票款或不低于()的旅行团费的票款。
正确答案:C
南航明珠牡丹信用卡为客户提供交通意外险服务,客户支付一定比例票款时,金卡和普通卡客户分别可获赠我行提供的()万元和()万元人民币的交通意外保险。
A.200;100
C.300;150
正确答案:B
财富客户成功申办牡丹白金卡,应给予该客户不低于()万元的信用额度。
正确答案:A
工银牡丹信用卡按信用等级可分为()。
A.贷记卡、准贷记卡
B.贷记卡、灵通卡
C.牡丹人民币贷记卡、牡丹双币贷记卡
D.白金卡、金卡、普通卡
正确答案:D
财富客户成功办理牡丹白金卡自第二年开始,客户在用卡年度内,若本外币账户当年累计消费额达到()万元人民币,则减免主副卡当年年费。
正确答案:B
客户持南航明珠牡丹信用卡消费,每消费1元人民币累计()积分,每消费1美元累计()积分,规定不累计积分的特定交易除外。
正确答案:C
我行南航明珠牡丹信用卡按月自动兑换南航奖励里程,兑换标准为每()个消费积分兑换1公里南航奖励里程。
正确答案:C
信用卡网上申请业务中营销代码前两位为()。
正确答案:C
在“网上办卡双重礼 自由选择新体验”营销活动期间通过网上途径申请的信用卡消费满8000元的客户额外赠送()积分
正在加载中,请稍后...提现申请,啥时候到银行卡倔π吩乒喊_百度知道
提现申请,啥时候到银行卡倔π吩乒喊
我有更好的答案
若是从微信钱包将资金提现到招行,到账时间取决于财付通系统的处理速度,正常情况下,到账时间以16点为界,16点之前提现的款项会在当天23点59分之前到账,16点之后提现的款项会在次日23点29分之前到账。
主营:金融
为您推荐:
其他类似问题
换一换
回答问题,赢新手礼包
个人、企业类
违法有害信息,请在下方选择后提交
色情、暴力
我们会通过消息、邮箱等方式尽快将举报结果通知您。&p&Blogger Ben Blum (online &i&Medium, &/i&June 7, 2018) recently questioned the authenticity and value of the 1971 Stanford Prison Experiment (SPE), labeling it a “fraud” and a “lie,” and other commentators have followed suit. For example, after watching a video that I deposited with the Stanford Archives, Brian Resnick wrote, “This damning video debunks the famed experiment” (VOX (June 14, 2018), and Jay Van Bavel told LIVE-SCIENCE (June 12, 2018), “The bottom line is that conformity isn't natural, blind or inevitable.” French author Thibault LeTextier (2018) even published a book-length critique entitled &i&History of a Lie.&/i&&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&In this response to my critics, I hereby assert that none of these criticisms present any substantial evidence that alters the SPE’s main conclusion concerning the importance of understanding how systemic and situational forces can operate to influence individual behavior in negative or positive directions, often without our personal awareness. The SPE’s core message is not that a psychological simulation of prison life is the same as the real thing, or that prisoners and guards always or even usually behave the way that they did in the SPE. Rather, the SPE serves as a cautionary tale of what &i&might&/i& happen to any of us if we underestimate the extent to which the power of social roles and external pressures can influence our actions.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Background&/i&&/p&&p&What was the Stanford Prison Experiment, and what serendipitous events catapulted this academic experiment about situational power into national prominence? The SPE was a study conducted at Stanford University over six days, August 14-19, 1971, designed and conducted by me, as principal investigator, along with my research team of graduate students, Craig Haney and William Curtis Banks, undergraduate David Jaffe, and prison consultant, Carlo Prescott. It was an exploratory investigation of the extent to which the power of situational forces could transform individual behaviors of participants. Twenty-four college students, recruited from a newspaper ad to participate in a study of prison life, first completed a battery of psychological tests and surveys (in order to establish that they were healthy and normal, and had not had any prior experience of breaking the law). These students were then randomly assigned the roles of prisoner and guard. The guards worked 8-hour shifts, while the prisoners lived full time in a mock prison setting created in the basement of the Stanford psychology department. Stanford’s Human Subjects Research Office approved this unique experiment, within the guidelines they provided. The intended two-week experiment was terminated after 6 days because of the unexpectedly extremely negative reactions of many of the mock guards and prisoners. Full details of this study are available in my book &i&The Lucifer Effect&/i& (2007) and online, &i&&u&&a href=&//link.zhihu.com/?target=http%3A//www.prisonexperiment.org/& class=& wrap external& target=&_blank& rel=&nofollow noreferrer&&www.PrisonExperiment.org&/a&&/u&.&/i& &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Shortly after the SPE ended, dramatic events in two American prisons—San Quentin and Attica—brought prison conditions into the national limelight. On August 20, during an alleged escape attempt by Black political prison activist George Jackson, a number of San Quentin guards and prisoners were killed. From September 9 - 13, a thousand Attica prisoners took control of that facility in public protest against Jackson’s “murder.” That confrontation ended with National Guardsmen killing many of those prisoners as well as their prison guard hostages. Extensive news coverage and congressional investigations ensued, and I was invited to participate in both media interviews and congressional hearings, which generated considerable interest in what had happened in our mock prison.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Before responding to questions about whether I portrayed the SPE honestly and accurately, it's important to note that I have gone to great lengths to make every bit of documentary information from the SPE publicly available in the archives of Stanford University and Akron University’s Museum of Psychology. This information includes more than 40 boxes o prisoner, guard, diaries that were gathered during and and 12 hours of videos made during the study. &i&The Lucifer Effect&/i& also contains 10 chapters devoted to various aspects of the SPE, with full documentation as to the source of every assertion. In addition, a considerable amount of material about the SPE has been available on the Stanford Prison Experiment website for more than 15 years. Thus, contrary to critics who imply that they've unearthed new information that I kept hidden, the SPE has been a model of open science long before practices such as public archiving and data sharing were common. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Critics also claim that my place in modern psychology is based primarily on the SPE. However, my reputation derives from considerable research and theories on many topics, both before and after the SPE. Indeed, I was already a tenured full professor at Stanford and nearly 40 years old by the time that the SPE was conducted in 1971, and it was my earlier work at New York University that led me to be invited to author one of the leading introductory psychology texts, &i&Psychology and Life &/i&(over 12 editions), and later &i&Psychology: Core Concepts.&/i& Subsequently, I was chosen from a group of textbook authors to become the creator and narrator of the 26-part TV series, &i&Discovering Psychology&/i&, which has been viewed by millions of students and teachers around the world. I suspect that my place within psychology derives as much from those contributions, if not more so, than it does from the SPE itself. Overall, I have contributed to 40 different areas of psychology, as documented in more than 60 books and 600 publications that I have written so far.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&In this reply, I will first address claims about the alleged fraudulent nature of the experiment’s process and conclusions. I will detail some of the study’s unique features, discuss its scientific validity, and describe several real world applications. Finally, I will conclude by outlining some unexpectedly valuable extensions derived from my SPE experiences and reflections. &/p&&p&&i&Main Criticisms&/i&&/p&&p&Recent criticisms of the SPE have focused on six issues, each of which I will address in turn. &/p&&p&&i&1. A staff member publicly denounced the SPE as flawed and dishonest. &/i&Blum cites a 2005 op-ed in the &i&Stanford Daily &/i&student newspaper allegedly written by SPE prison consultant Carlo Prescott and entitled “The lie of the Stanford Prison Experiment.” In fact, Blum even borrowed the op-ed title as the core theme for his blog. The reality is that Carlo never wrote a word of that op-ed. He and I had become friends after our first meeting in my social psychology class in May 1971, and after I learned that he had served time in prison, I invited him to serve as SPE's expert advisor on prison life. A careful reading of the student newspaper op-ed makes evident that the writer had a very distinctive legalistic style and vocabulary, not at all like Carlo’s. It turns out that its real author, who also published many related negative SPE comments online, was Michael Lazarou, a Los Angeles movie writer. He had befriended both Carlo and me in an attempt to get me to agree to give him screen rights to a Hollywood movie about the SPE. When I chose instead to go with Maverick Films producer Brent Emery, Lazarou began writing critiques of the SPE (Brent Emery’s phone records indicated, “Carlo said it was NOT him, but all from Lazarou,” May 7, 2005). In other words, it is simply not the case that the SPE prison consultant referred to the study as a lie.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&2. The staff's instructions for guards to be &tough& biased the guards' behavior and distorted the research outcomes. &/i&The SPE was designed as a mock prison simulating some of the main features that characterized the American prison system at that time. Central in the training of guards was to exercise their power over the prisoners so that they maintained order, prevented rebellion, and eliminated escape attempts. My instructions to the guards, as documented by recordings of the guard orientation, were that they could not hit the prisoners but could create feelings of boredom, frustration, fear, and &a sense of powerlessness—that is, we have total power of the situation, and they have none.& We did not give any formal or detailed instructions about &i&how&/i& to be an effective guard.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&None of the participants wanted to a guard, but half were randomly assigned to that role. As shown in a documentary film on the SPE, entitled &i&Quiet Rage&/i&, the guards took awhile to videos from the first day show them giggling while encouraging prisoners to take the rules seriously. One of three guards on a shift that day wasn't even getting prisoners to follow orders issued by the other guards, so David Jaffee, acting as the SPE Warden, took this guard aside and asked him to become more active, involved, and &tough& in order to make the experimental setting seem more like a prison. Here are his exact words:&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&We really want to get you active and involved because the guards have to know that every guard is going to be what we call a “tough guard”… what I mean by tough is [that] you have to be firm, and you have to be in the action… It’s really important… for the workings of the experiment [because] whether or not we can make this thing seem like a prison—which is the aim of the thing—depends largely on the guards’ behavior.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&There was never any further specification in &i&how &/i&to be active or tough, although there was an explicit admonition against any use of physical force. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Despite the request David Jaffee made, none of the guards behaved in a dominant way during the first two shifts. What made a difference was that on Day 2 the prisoners rebelled with verbal and physical confrontations challenging the full complement of nine guards. After the guards put down this rebellion, one of them declared that prisoners were &dangerous,& and with that new view of the situation, several guards became much tougher in their actions. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&It's important to note that in all my reports about the SPE, I have always highlighted individual differences among the guards. One or two guards on each shift became progressively meaner over time, others maintained a more even-tempered style, and a few were considered “good guards” from the prisoners’ perspectives. However, none of the “good guards” ever intervened to prevent the cruelty of their fellow guards. Even Blum acknowledges these individual differences among the guards. From my perspective, the range of guard behaviors undercuts any criticisms of the alleged demand characteristics that presumably distorted the results of the SPE. The fact that some guards remained &good guards& throughout the study shows that cruel guards chose to act on their own initiative. It is their extreme behaviors that generated the dramatic effects of the study, most notably those of the iconic guard nicknamed “John Wayne” for his macho performance.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&3. One guard was intentionally play-acting his role. &/i&The prisoners nicknamed one guard on the night shift “John Wayne” because he acted like an out-of-control, Wild West cowboy. However, some critics have dismissed this guard's behavior as merely play-acting the role of tough guard. After the experiment, “John Wayne” (David Eshelman) explained that he modeled his role after the warden in the movie &i&Cool Hand Luke. &/i&He said he wanted to be a realistic guard, so he stepped up to lead his night shift to be really tough on the prisoners. He did so by punishing prisoners repeatedly with extensive push-ups (occasionally, with some prisoners stepping on the back of others), limiting food access, or issuing arbitrary rules. With each passing night, he became more creatively evil in ways that went beyond being a tough guard. Indeed, he later said that he began to think of himself as a “puppeteer” who made prisoners do whatever he chose. In an extreme perversion of his experimentally assigned role, he devised an unthinkable way to humiliate all prisoners on the fifth night of the study. He ordered them to think of themselves as “camels,” half as males and the other half as females. Those ordered to be female camels had to bend over, while the male camel prisoners were ordered to hump them “doggy style,” which they reluctantly did by simulating sodomy. A video recording, made in my absence, indicated this episode lasted nearly ten minutes with all three guards shouting epithets and laughing hysterically. Fortunately, I had earlier decided to terminate the experiment the next morning. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&I think it goes without saying that such actions go far beyond simply playing the role of a tough guard. It is also worth noting that Eshelman's fellow guards fully participated in these activities and other offensive behaviors that typified their shift—activities that were strikingly similar to the sexually degrading rituals imposed on Iraqi prisoners by American prison guards in Abu Ghraib Prison. Moreover, the night shift was not alone in excessively brutalizing the SPE several guards on the other two shifts also regularly engaged in acts designed to humiliate the prisoners. Were these acts of brutality—and their striking parallels with real-world prison atrocities—nothing more than a function of social demand characteristics in a fraudulent &sham& study, as Blum and other critics have argued, or do they tell us something important about human nature? The full body of available evidence clearly suggests the latter.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&
4. &i&A prisoner who seemed to have an emotional breakdown was actually just faking the breakdown to leave the study early.&/i& Blum portrays the case of Doug Korpi, alias Prisoner 8612, as an instance of me being duped into believing that a prisoner was having an emotional breakdown when in fact the prisoner was simply faking a breakdown in order to leave the study early. The evidence Blum cites for this conclusion is that Korpi told Blum in an interview: &I was faking… If you listen to the tape, you can hear it in my voice... I was being a good employee. It was a great time.” To this criticism, I have two responses. First, I would argue that any researcher who believes a research participant is having a breakdown is ethically obliged to treat the breakdown as real, even if the breakdown later turns out to be feigned. And second, I'm not alone in regarding the breakdown as real, because Doug Korpi himself went on record in &i&Quiet Rage&/i& as saying that his time as a prisoner was the most upsetting experience of his life (see the accompanying video).&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&For reasons I cannot fathom, Korpi’s story has changed several times over the past 47 years: from genuinely losing control of his emotions, to getting out of the study so that he could lead an insurrection and liberate the other prisoners, to faking a breakdown just to get out early and study for an upcoming Graduate Record Exam, and to other reflections and memory distortions. Regardless, the conclusions Korpi drew from the study in his &i&Quiet Rage&/i& interview, 17 years after the experience, are fully consistent with my own conclusions: &The Stanford Prison was a very benign prison situation, and it still caused guards to become sadistic [and] prisoners to become hysterical.&&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&5. A British research team failed to replicate the SPE. &/i&An “experiment” based loosely on the SPE was filmed and broadcast on a 4-part BBC-TV show in May 2002 (Koppel & Mirsky, 2002). Its results appeared to challenge those of the SPE because the guards showed little violence or cruelty toward the prisoners. Instead, the opposite occurred. The prisoners dominated the guards, to the point where the guards became “increasingly paranoid, depressed and stressed and complained most of being bullied.” Several of the guards couldn’t take it any more and quit: none of the prisoners did so. Blum points to that TV show as another challenge to the validity of the SPE. However, in no way did this “reality-show” meet the scientific criteria for a replication. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&From the time of being recruited with national ads to be actors in a &university-backed social science experiment to be shown on TV,& every participant knew their actions and voices (from lapel mics they had to wear always) would be seen and heard on national TV by family and colleagues. Any similarity to the intense build-up of emotional confrontations between SPE guards versus prisoners, 24/7, was diluted by the daily itinerary of the British research team (Alex Haslam and Stephen Reicher). These researchers frequently intervened, made regular public broadcasts into the prison facility, administered daily psychological assessments, arranged contests for the best prisoners to compete to become guards, and as in many “reality-TV” shows, created daily &confessionals& for participants to talk directly to the camera about their feelings. Ironically, the results of this show could be interpreted as further evidence of the “power of the situation,” although in this case the “situation” was that of reality-TV.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Among the participants in this BBC-TV prison show, several of whom had contacted me afterwards, was Philip Bimpson, the ringleader of a prisoner rebellion against the hapless guards. He said, in part:&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&The prisoners won because they had organized themselves qui their subversive actions and organizational skills outwitted the guards who were disorganized in their new surroundings. They did not understand that they had to organize themselves and form a set of rules that they all agreed on… I think the group is being exploited by the BBC for commercial gain. Me and my new friends in the group joined the experiment for the furtherance of science & not to be used as a form of cheap entertainment.& (Personal email communication, 26 Feb. 2002; supplemented by my subsequent visit in Glasgow, Oct. 10, 2004)&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&I therefore reject the use of this “replication” as a scientifically valid challenge to results from the SPE (for a more detailed response to this criticism, please see my article in &i&The British Journal of Social Psychology&/i&, 2007, Vol. 127.).&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&6. Early publications appeared in non-peer reviewed journals to avoid rejection. &/i&Several critics have claimed that I chose to publish early accounts of the SPE in non-peer reviewed journals in order to avoid likely rejection of this study. That is not the case. The research team published its first account of the SPE in the &i&Journal of the Office of Naval Research&/i& because I had used funds from ONR that were left over from a previous grant, and ONR insisted that I document my research in its journal. Our next publication was in the &i&International Journal of Penology and Criminology, &/i&at the invitation of the editor. In 1973, I then published an account of the SPE in the &i&New York Times Magazine&/i&—not to bypass peer review, but to reach a large national audience and use the opportunity to frame the SPE as a &i&Pirandellian Prison&/i&. My colleagues and I subsequently published several other articles and chapters about the SPE in peer-reviewed journals and books for academic audiences, including the &i&American Psychologist&/i&, which is rigorously peer-reviewed.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Additional Topics&/i& &/p&&p&Now I want to turn to some other issues: highlighting what is unique about the SPE; describing its scientific and co showing how it has influenced decisio and discovering how the U.S. military has applied one of its main conclusions. I then turn to reflect on three of its enduring positive extensions in the domains of shyness, time perspective, and heroism.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Uniqueness of the SPE&/i&&/p&&p&The design of the SPE is unique in social psychological research by enabling observations of behavior patterns of the participants during an extended period of more than 120 hours. One of its main conclusions is that participants who were randomly assigned to roles of prisoner or guard gradually assumed those new identities in a simulated prison setting, despite their awareness of its experimental nature. Most other research is typically compressed into a one-hour session, so it is not possible to observe the emergence of situated identities, such as some normal, healthy college students becoming either cruel guards or helpless prisoners. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Scientific Validation&/i&&/p&&p&Replication with variations by independent researchers is the hallmark of all experimental research, and so it was with the SPE. A team of researchers at the University of New South Wales, Australia, extended the SPE design by having one condition similar to ours and several other experimental variants to explore how social organizations influence the relationship between prisoners and guards (Lovibond, Mithiran, & Adams, 1979). Their &Standard Custodial& regime was modeled on medium security prisons in Australia and was closest in its procedure to the SPE. The researchers' central conclusion of their rigorous experimental protocol notes: &Our results thus support the major conclusion of Zimbardo et al. that hostile, confrontive relations in prisons result primarily from the nature of the prison regime, rather than the personal characteristics of inmates and officers.& (pp. 283). These results, within this research design, also help offset skepticism about the validity of such simulation experiments by providing baselines to assess behavioral changes from objectively defined structural characteristics of real-life prisons. However, it should be noted that it is no longer possible for anyone to attempt a full replication of the SPE, given the critiques that it is not ethical to ever conduct this type of research again.
&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Conceptual Validation: The Mock Psychiatric Ward as SPE for Staff&/i&&/p&&p&Consulting with research director, Norma Jean Orlando, I advised on how it would be possible to create a mock psychiatric ward in Elgin State Mental Hospital in Illinois, where 29 staff members played the roles of mental patients on a locked ward for 3 days and nights. Twenty-two regular staff played their usual roles, while trained observers and video recordings reported on all that transpired. In a short time, most mock patients began behaving in ways that were indistinguishable from real patients: six tried to escape, two withdrew into themselves, others were totally silent, two wept uncontrollably, another came close to having a nervous breakdown, and the majority reported feeling “incarcerated” with no one caring about their well-being. One staff member-turned- patient who suffered during the weekend ordeal gained enough insight to declare: “ I used to look at the patients as if they wer I never knew what they were going through before.” The positive outcome of this experience was the formation of a committee of staff members working cooperatively with current and former patients who were dedicated to raising consciousness of hospital personnel about the way patients were being mistreated and how they had to change their behavior to create a more constructive, humane environment (Orlando 1973).&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&The Enduring Value of SPE’s Message in Legal Settings&/i&&/p&&p&I have spent much of my professional career trying to bridge the translation of research findings into social change efforts that promote social justice and what is best in human nature. In their hearings on prison reform (October, 1971), the Subcommittee of the United States Congress House of Representatives wanted not only my analyses, but also recommendations for reform. In my statement in the &i&Congressional Record, &/i&I clearly advocated for congressional intervention into the prison structure to bring about improvements in the condition of inmates, as well as for correctional personnel. In addition, I wish here to highlight the important contributions that Craig Haney has made to prison reform. He is widely regarded as the foremost expert in the nation on the psychology of imprisonment, notably in challenging conditions of solitary confinement and the death penalty. Craig’s expertise is based on having earned both a Ph.D. in psychology and a law degree from Stanford University.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Unfortunately, blogger Blum’s &i&Medium&/i& critique misrepresents published testimony in the Congressional Record about my constructive arguments for improving our nation’s mass incarceration system. My advocacy has largely taken the form of consciousness-raising about the necessity for ending the “social experiment” of prisons because, as measured by high rates of recidivism and current mass levels of incarceration in the United States, that experiment has failed. We must find the reason for that through more thorough systems analyses, and propose alternative solutions to incarceration. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&My second testimony before a Congressional subcommittee that focused on juvenile detention (September, 1973) moved me further toward becoming a social advocate. I outlined 19 separate recommendations for improved treatment of detained juveniles. I was pleased to learn that a new federal law was passed that was in part stimulated by my testimony. Senator Birch Bayh, who headed this investigation, helped to put into law the rule that, to prevent juveniles being abused, juveniles in pre-trial detention should not be housed with adults in federal prisons. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&One powerful legal impact of the SPE derived from my participation in the Federal Court trial of Spain &i&et al. &/i&versus Procunier &i&et al.&/i& (1973). The &San Quentin Six& prisoners had been isolated in solitary confinement for several years for their alleged involvement in the murder of guards and informer prisoners during the escape attempt of George Jackson on August 21, 1971. As an expert witness, I toured the facilities of San Quentin's maximum adjustment center and interviewed each of the six prisoners a number of times. My prepared statement and two days of trial testimony concluded with the opinion that all of these prison conditions of involuntary, prolonged, indefinite confinement under dehumanizing conditions constitutes &cruel and unusual punishment& and must therefore be changed. The Court arrived at a similar conclusion and ordered improved living conditions for inmates. In addition, I served throughout the trial as a psychological consultant to the team of lawyers for the plaintiffs. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Later, in 2004, I was asked to be an expert witness in the military trial of American prison guard, Staff Sgt. Chip Frederick. He was a leader of the guards on the night shift in Abu Ghraib Prison, all of whom participated in brutalizing Iraqi prisoners. There were many apparent similarities between the incidents of prisoner abuse by the guards in Abu Ghraib and the SPE. Frederick’s behaviors were shown to be completely atypical, as he had no prior history of such harmful actions towards any other people, and instead had been given many honors for his outstanding military service. Although he admitted his guilt in committing these abuses in Abu Ghraib, his prison sentence was shortened considerably by the judge’s acknowledgement of my testimony documenting the power of the situation in that unusual prison.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Interestingly, I was also asked to be an expert witness in the federal trial of Alex Blum, the cousin of Ben Blum, who mentions him in his &i&Medium &/i&blog. Briefly, Alex was a guilty party as get-away driver in a bank robbery along with three other U.S. Rangers. Alex had long obsessed about becoming a U.S. Ranger, and as a recruit, he was assigned to an officer, Luke Elliot Sommer, who was his taskmaster and who issued orders to be obeyed without question. One of them was to assist in a “practice drill” of robbing a local bank, but in fact, it turned out to be a full-scale bank robbery. I spent enough personal time with Alex to realize the extent to which he was “blindly obeying& the authority of Capt. Sommer (who got a 24-year prison sentence). Alex might have received a similar long sentence, but instead it was reduced to only 16 months, presumably because the court accepted the expert testimony. When in prison, it took Alex a full eight months to come to the realization that what he had done was a real bank robbery and not a ranger drill, which for me demonstrated the power of his “Ranger-Mindset” of total obedience to his assigned authority. It was a powerful combination of Milgram’s obedience scenario along with cult-leader mind control, as seen in the Jonestown followers of Jim Jones, and those of Rev. Moon. For valuable information on mind control, see Steven Hassan’s &i&Combating Cult Mind Control&/i& (2015).&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Situationally-Based Behavioral Explanations Are Never Forms of “Excusiology”&/i&&/p&&p&“What social psychology has given to an understanding of human nature is the discovery that forces larger than ourselves determine our mental life and our actions—chief among these forces is the power of the social situation” (Banaji, 2001, P&i&sychological Science Agenda&/i&). &/p&&p&In Solomon Asch’s classic research (1955), the power of a group majority distorted the perceptual judgments of individual college students. In Stanley Milgram’s obedience research (1963), the power of an authority figure induced actions that went against the moral conscience of adult male participants to harm a stranger. In experimental research on moral disengagement, Albert Bandura (1975) showed that college student participants shocked the “errors” of other students with highest intensity when they had been labeled “animals,” compared to other conditions. In the SPE, we witnessed the creation over time of two mentalities, that of dominating guards and of helpless, hopeless prisoners in a setting that validated these alternative personas. I believe that these four psychological studies, among others in our discipline, illustrate the extent to which the power of a social situation can come to dominate and distort individual perceptions, judgments, values and behaviors.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&It should be made crystal-clear that when social psychologists attempt to explain the behavior of individuals in terms of influential external situational forces, they are &i&never &/i&implying that personal responsibility is absolved. People are always responsible for the consequences of their actions -- personally, socially and legally. Understanding &i&why &/i&we do something does not excuse our liability for the outcomes of that behavior. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Nevertheless, some critics of the SPE display a na?ve misunderstanding of this perspective by claiming that a message of the SPE is that individuals &cannot really be held accountable for the sometimes reprehensible things we do . . . it is also profoundly liberating. It means we're off the hook” (Blum, 2018). Similarly, LeTextier (2018) proclaims: “It’s like, ‘Oh my god, I could be a Nazi myself. I thought I was a good guy, and now I discover that I could be this monster.’ And in the meantime, it’s quite reassuring, because if I become a monster, it’s not because deep inside me I am the devil, it’s because of the situation. I think that’s why the (SPE) experiment was so famous in Germany and Eastern Europe. You don’t feel guilty. ‘Oh, okay, it was the situation. We are all good guys. No problem. It’s just the situation made us do it.’ So it’s shocking, but at the same time it’s reassuring.”&/p&&p&This argument was rejected in the Nuremberg trials of Nazi doctors and others many decades ago,
the individuals were indeed “just doing their job” but were still held accountable for the atrocities they committed. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&I strongly reject the criticism that the underlying message of the SPE is to absolve people of their “sins.” Changing or preventing undesirable behavior of individuals or groups requires an understanding of what strengths, virtues, and vulnerabilities that they bring into any given situation. We need to recognize more fully the complex of situational forces that are operative in given behavioral settings. Modifying them, or learning to avoid them, can have a greater impact on reducing undesirable individual reactions than remedial actions directed only at changing the people in a situation after they have done wrong. That means adopting a public health approach in place of the standard medical model approach to curing individual ills and wrongs. I have stated repeatedly that attempting to understand the situational and systemic contributions to any individual's behavior does not excuse the person or absolve him or her from responsibility in engaging in immoral, illegal, or evil deeds. Furthermore, I've always endorsed all efforts to create conditions, systemic and social, which can bring out the best in human nature&i&.&/i&&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Military Use of SPE in SERE Trainings&/i&&/p&&p&When I lectured at the United States Naval Academy, I was informed that the Navy and other military units train their personnel using the documentary footage of the SPE to avoid the excesses that are likely to occur during these exercises. Following the Korean War, when some airmen were alleged to have given actionable information to the enemy, our military developed a policy of never giving any information when captured by any enemy, other than name, rank and serial number. The military instituted war games in which some personnel acted as escaped prisoners who were hunted down by other staff, and then interrogated as intensely as possible in order to break them down into giving confessions and vital information. This program, Search, Evasion, Resistance, Escape (SERE) continues to function as one effective training program to achieve the desired objective of never complying with any enemy commands for information. However, there have been reports of excesses practiced by the simulating interrogators, which were dangerous to the well-being of the simulating captives. The SPE is shown as a warning of the ease in which anyone can cross the line from play-acting to becoming cruel torturers. I found this to be an unexpected positive outcome of some of SPE’s messages in real-world settings.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&SPE as Pirandellian Prison&/i&&/p&&p&The opportunity to contribute an article to the &i&New York Times Magazine&/i& (1973) allowed our research team to present our experience to a larger public audience, rather than limit it to academics only. This meant that we had to engage in a different style of writing. I valued the idea of framing the SPE in terms of playwright Luigi Pirandello's metaphors of the thin line between reality and illusion, between acting and being, between role-playing and the role becoming the self, and so that is what we did. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&The SPE was a drama that was enacted by young men playing their assigned roles without scripted lines and without an audience for hours and days on end. Everyone knew it was only a play. Everyone knew it was just a p that they were in the basement of the psych department, that the prisoners had not
that the guards could have earned their salary by simply playing cards in their guard office for much of their 8-hour shift as long as they kept prisoners locked in their cells. But in a relatively short time frame, the psychology study became a “prison,” and the only two ways out of that dungeon were to be released by the arbitrary decision of the parole board or by becoming/acting seriously disturbed mentally or physically. In addition to Korpi, four other mock prisoners had to be released early for extreme emotional or medical reasons.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&As is also apparent from our documentary videos and verbatim transcripts, not only did most of the research participants enact their roles as if they were imprisoned or were doing their job as paid guards, but almost everyone else who got engaged in that setting acted as if it were a real prison. The prime example is the Catholic priest who I had invited to evaluate the fit of this experiment with his experience in real prisons. He did so by interviewing all the prisoners. However, he soon got into the role of prisoner counseler, calling a prisoner’s mother to inform her that her son needed a public defender to help get him out of this prison. The public defender also knew the SPE was just an experiment, but when meeting with the desperate prisoners, he maintained his aloof role that was legally limited to what he could do to help them. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Carlo Prescott got so involved in his role as head of the parole board that he began to chastise and verbally abuse many of those pleading to be paroled, until some cried and begged for his understanding of their case. He later said that he had unconsciously become the kind of person he had hated most in the world, the parole board authorities that had turned down his own requests dozens of times before. Afterwards, he reported becoming sick to his stomach, realizing what he had become, and he did not want to play that role ever again. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&And then there was me. I began the study as the principal research investigator, in charge of my team of student researchers, all interested in exploring together the dynamics of a unique situation we had created. I had initially instituted observational and research protocols, video recording assignments, and data collection procedures, as all experimental researchers do. However, over a very short time, I was transformed into the full-time role of Prison Superintendent. In my view, that is the major flaw of the SPE; it had no independent scientific observer of the unfolding events. My agenda became less about data collection than about daily staff assignments, timing of meals, meeting with concerned parents, parole board sessions, guard shift changes, meeting with the prisoner grievance committee, dealing with prisoner breakdowns, and more. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&One vivid illustration of this flaw in the study came on Thursday night, five days into the study, when there was the usual 10:00 pm scheduled toilet run -- the last time prisoners could go to a regular toilet rather than urinate or defecate in buckets in their cells. The night shift guards used this event as an opportunity to torment and confuse the prisoners in various ways. By then, I no longer acknowledged the suffer the toilet run was only a checkmark on my daily schedule. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&That's when I received an unexpected challenge from a visitor observing how the guards were dehumanizing and tormenting the prisoners (again without any staff direction). Christina Maslach, a new psychology professor at U.C. Berkeley, whom I was dating, came down to take me out to dinner. However, when she saw the ongoing treatment of the hooded, chained prisoners, she ran outside in tears. We had a major confrontation in which she made clear that this situation had changed &i&me&/i& from a teacher who loved students into someone who could be indifferent to student suffering. She then said she didn't want to continue our relationship if I did not come to my senses. That was my wake-up call to shed the Prison Superintendent garb, return to my usual persona, and terminate the SPE the next morning. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&So what really happened in the five days of the SPE? Was it an unfolding drama of human nature in its worst apparel, or just kids play-acting to please the director?&/p&&p&I strongly believe the former. &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Enduring Legacies &/i& &/p&&p&Three of my contributions to psychology and society have emerged in various ways from extensions of ideas I extracted from the SPE. They are: (1) understanding and treating shyness, (2) understanding and utilizing the power of time perspective to improve the quality of our lives, and (3) creating the &i&Heroic Imagination Project &/i&to inspire compassionate social action for good and against evil.
&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Shyness.&/i& I first conceptualized shyness in 1972 as a self-imposed psychological prison in which the shy individual plays the dual roles of guard (who limits all freedom of speech and social behavior) and also the reluctant prisoner (who submits to those constraints and thus loses much self-esteem). I went on to do pioneering research on this topic, created the first shyness treatment clinic (still in operation at Palo Alto University), and also published several popular books and magazine articles for the general public. Thus, I have integrated education, research, therapy, and public awareness of this widespread personal and social phenomenon.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Time Perspective.&/i& My interest in exploring the psychology of time perspective, or the temporal zones in which we all live, emerged in part from the sense of distorted time we all experienced during the SPE. Without clocks or windows, that basement prison’s time revolved around each guard shift coming and going. We often felt trapped in an expanded present time zone when the guards were endlessly harassing the prisoners, or in a present fatalistic time zone that most prisoners experienced when nothing they did made a difference in how they were treated. I subsequently developed a scale to measure individual differences in time perspective, the Zimbardo Time Perspective I conducted original r published several books on time perspective and its applications in various kinds of therapy both to heal trauma and to improve the quality of our lives. In addition, I have brought together a global team of researchers and practitioners studying, using, and extending the ideas and procedures that emerged in part from one of my many personal experiences during the SPE.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Creating Everyday Heroes&/i&. In the final chapter of &i&The Lucifer Effect&/i&, I switched focus from trying to understand how good people can turn evil to asking whether it is possible for ordinary people to be inspired and trained to become everyday heroes. Since then, and with support of colleagues following my public statement of that theme in my 2008 TED talk, &i&On the Psychology of Evil&/i&, I've devoted my academic and personal life to creating a nonprofit foundation, The Heroic Imagination Project (HIP). Its mission is to inspire and train ordinary people, especially our youth, to be ready and willing to enact extraordinary deeds of compassion in challenging situations they face in their lives. In addition to a research agenda, we have developed a unique educational program that provides the foundation for training people how to think and act heroically in situations they face. These lessons have demonstrated effectiveness with high school and college students, as well as in business settings. HIP has become a global movement with vibrant programs in more than a dozen nations on several continents, with many more coming on board soon. For more information, please visit &i&&u&&a href=&//link.zhihu.com/?target=http%3A//www.heroicimagination.org/& class=& wrap external& target=&_blank& rel=&nofollow noreferrer&&www.HeroicImagination.org&/a&&/u&&/i& &/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&Conclusion&/i&&/p&&p&I hope that this reply to the critics of the legitimacy and enduring value of the SPE help make evident that they are substantially wrong in their conclusions. For whatever its flaws, I continue to believe that the Stanford Prison Experiment contributes to psychology’s understanding of human behavior and its complex dynamics. Multiple forces shape human behavior, they are internal and external, historical and contemporary,&/p&&p&cultural and personal. The more we understand all of these dynamics and the complex way they interact with each other, the better we will be at promoting what is best in human nature. That has been my lifelong mission.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Selected References&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&CRITICS&/i&&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Blum, B. (2018, June 7). The Lifespan of a Lie. &i&Medium.&/i&&/p&&p&Resnick, B. (2018, June 14). This damning video debunks the famed experiment. &i&VOX.&/i&&/p&&p&Van Bavel, J. V. (2018, June 12). “The bottom line is that conformity isn't natural, blind or inevitable.” &i&Live SCI=NCE.&/i&&/p&&p&Textier, T. (2018). &i&History of a Lie&/i&. Paris: Editions La Decouverte.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&REBUTTAL CITATIONS&/i&&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Asch, S. (1955). Opinions and Social Pressure. &i&Scientific American, 193&/i& (5), 31-35.&/p&&p&Bandura, A., Underwood, B., & Fromson, M. E. (1975). Disinhibition of aggression through diffusion of responsibility and dehumanization of victims. &i&Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 253-269.&/i&&/p&&p&Blass, T. (1991). Understanding behavior in the Milgram obedience experiment: The role of personality, situations, and their interactions. &i&Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60&/i&, 398- 413.&/p&&p&Blum, B. (2017). &i&Ranger Games&/i&. New York: Doubleday. &/p&&p&Lovibond, S. H., Mithiran, X., and Adams,W.G. (1979). The effects of three experimental prison environments on the behaviour of non-convict volunteer subjects. &i&Australian Psychologist, 14, &/i&273-87.&/p&&p&Milgram, S. (1974). &i&Obedience to authority: An experimental view. &/i&New York: Harper & Row.&/p&&p&Orlando, N.J. (1973). &i&The Mock Ward: A study in Simulation&/i&. In Behavior Disorders: Perspectives and Trends. O. Milton & G. Whalers, Eds. (3rd Ed., Philadelphia: Lippincott) pp. 162-170.&/p&&p&Zimbardo. P. G. (2007). &i&British J. Social Psychology, 127: Commentary for BPS Journals Department. Rethinking the psychology of tyranny: The BBC Prison Study&/i&&/p&&p&Wells, M. (Jan. 24, 2002). BBC halts 'prison experiment.& &i&The Guardian Unlimited&/i&, on line.&/p&&p&Koppel, G., & Mirsky, N. (Series producer and Executive producer, respectively.). (2002, May, 14,15, 20, 21). &i&The Experiment&/i&. London: BBC.&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G. (1970). The human choice: Individuation, reason, and order versus deindividuation, impulse, and chaos. In W. J. Arnold & D. Levine (Eds.), &i&1969 Nebraska Symposium on Motivation&/i& (pp. 237-307). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Zimbardo, P. G. (1971). The power and pathology of imprisonment. &i&Congressional Record&/i&. (Serial No. 15, October 25, 1971). Hearings before Subcommittee No. 3, of the Committee on the Judiciary, House of Representatives, Ninety-Second Congress, &i&First Session on Corrections, Part II, Prisons, Prison Reform and Prisoner’s Rights: California&/i&. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.Zimbardo, P. G. (1972). Pathology of Imprisonment. &i&Society, 6,&/i& 4, 6, 8.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&THE SPE&/i&&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Haney, C., Banks, W. C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1973). Interpersonal dynamics in a simulated prison. &i&International Journal of Criminology and Penology, 1&/i&, 69-97.Haney, C., Banks, W. C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1973). &i&Study of prisoners and guards in a simulated prison &/i&(Naval Research Reviews 9 (1-17)). Washington, DC: Office of Naval Research.Zimbardo, P. G., Haney, C., Banks, W. C., & Jaffe, D. (1973, April 8). The mind is a formidable jailer: A Pirandellian prison. &i&The New York Times Magazine&/i&, Section 6, pp. 38, ff.Zimbardo, P. G., Haney, C., Banks, W. C., & Jaffe, D. (1974). The psychology of imprisonment: Privation, power and pathology. In Z. Rubin (Ed.), &i&Doing Unto Others: Explorations in Social Behavior&/i& (pp. 61-73). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Zimbardo, P. G. (1974). &i&The detention and jailing of juveniles&/i& (Hearings before U.S. Senate Committee on the Judiciary Subcommittee to Investigate Juvenile Delinquency, 10, 11, 17, September, 1973). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office, 141-161.Haney, C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1976). Social roles and role-playing: Observations from the Stanford prison study. In E. P. Hollander & R. G. Hunt (Eds.), &i&Current Perspectives In Social Psychology&/i& (4th ed.), (pp. 266-274). New York: Oxford University Press.&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G., & Haney, C. (1978). Prison behavior. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), &i&International Encyclopedia of Psychiatry, Psychology, Psychoanalysis and Neurology, &/i&(&i&Vol. 9&/i&; pp. 70-74). New York: Human Sciences Press.&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G. (1979). (Testimony of Dr. Philip Zimbardo to U.S. House of Representatives Committee on the Judiciary). In J. J. Bonsignore, et al. (Eds.), &i&Before the law: An introduction to the legal process&/i& (2nd ed.; pp. 396-399). Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Zimbardo, P. G. (1989). (Writer and producer), & Musen, K. (Co-writer and co-producer). Q&i&uiet Rage: The Stanford Prison Study&/i& [Video]. Stanford, CA: Stanford Instructional Television Network. &/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G., Maslach, C., & Haney, C. (1999). Reflections on the Stanford Prison Experiment: Genesis, transformations, consequences. In T. Blass (Ed.), &i&Obedience to Authority: Current Perspectives on the Milgram Paradigm&/i&. (pp. 193-237). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. &/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G., Plous, S., &, Lestik, M. (1999, Dec.). &i&The Stanford Prison Experiment Web Site&/i&: &u&&a href=&//link.zhihu.com/?target=http%3A//www.prisonexp.org& class=& external& target=&_blank& rel=&nofollow noreferrer&&&span class=&invisible&&http://www.&/span&&span class=&visible&&prisonexp.org&/span&&span class=&invisible&&&/span&&/a&&/u&.&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G., & Lestik, M. (2007). T&i&he Lucifer Effect Web Site&/i&. Launched March 2007. Web site: &u&&a href=&//link.zhihu.com/?target=http%3A//www.lucifereffect.com/& class=& wrap external& target=&_blank& rel=&nofollow noreferrer&&www.LuciferEffect.com&/a&&/u&.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&MIND CONTROL&/i&&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Scheflin, A.W., & Opton, E. M., Jr. (1978). &i&The mind manipulators: A non-fiction account&/i&. New York: Paddington Press.&/p&&p&Sullivan, D., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1979, March 9). Jonestown survivors tell their story. &i&Los Angeles Times, View section, Part 4&/i&, pp. 1, 10-12.&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G. (1983, Winter). To control a mind. &i&Stanford Magazine, 11&/i&, pp. 59-64.&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G., & Hartley, C. F. (1985, Spring/Summer). Cults go to high school: A theoretical and empirical analysis of the initial stage in the recruitment process. &i&Cultic Studies Journal, 2&/i&, 91-147. Weston, MA: American Family Foundation.&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G., & Andersen, S. A. (1993). Understanding mind control: Exotic and mundane mental manipulations. In M. Langone (Ed.), &i&Recovery from Cults&/i& (pp. 104-125). New York: Norton Press.&/p&&p&Layton D. (2003) &i&Seductive poison: A Jonestown survivor’s story of life and death in the peoples Temple. &/i&New York: Doubleday.&/p&&p&Hassan, S. (2015). &i&Combatting Cult Mind Control.&/i& Newton MA: Freedom of Mind Press.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&ACADEMIC CONTRIBUTIONS&/i&&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Ruch, F. L., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1971). &i&Psychology and Life&/i& (8th ed.). Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.Gerrig, R., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2015). &i&Psychology and Life&/i& (19th ed.). New York: Pearson.&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G., Johnson, R., & McCann, V. (2017). &i&Psychology: Core Concepts. &/i&(8TH ed.)&i&. &/i&NY: Pearson.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G. (1975). On transforming experimental research into advocacy for social change. In M. Deutsch & H. Hornstein (Eds.), &i&Applying Social Psychology: Implications For Research, Practice, and Training&/i& (pp. 33-66). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. &/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G. (1989). (Co-writer, chief academic advisor, host), & WGBH-TV (Producer). &i&Discovering Psychology&/i& [26-program video series.] Washington, DC: Annenberg-CPB. [Updated 2002].Zimbardo, P. G. (1999). Recollections of a social psychologist's career: An interview with Dr. Philip Zimbardo. &i&Journal of Social Behavior and personality, 14, &/i&1-22.&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). Does psychology make a significant difference in our lives? A&i&merican Psychologist, 59&/i&, 339-351.&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&&i&SHYNESS&/i&&/p&&p&&br&&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G., Pilkonis, P., & Norwood, R. (1975, May). The silent prison of shyness. &i&Psychology Today&/i&, 69-70, 72.Zimbardo, P. G. (1977). &i&Shyness: What It Is, What To Do About It&/i&. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.&/p&&p&Zimbardo, P. G. (1977, Jan.-Feb.). Shyness--The people phobia. &i&Today’s Education, 66&/i&, 47-49.Pines, A., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1978). The personal and cul}

我要回帖

更多关于 支付宝提现要手续费吗 的文章

更多推荐

版权声明:文章内容来源于网络,版权归原作者所有,如有侵权请点击这里与我们联系,我们将及时删除。

点击添加站长微信